Antioxidants and Autophagy

http://fightaging.org/archives/2009/05/exercise-reactive-oxygen-species-and-antioxidants.php

Mitochondrial reaction to ROS (“free radicals”) is part of how we gain increased work capacity through exercise.

Some guy recently showed a 30% increase in mouse lifespan with some mitochondrial-ROS-protective drug (which you can’t obtain yet).  This would probably come at the cost of decreased adaptation to exercise.

Most antioxidants do a terrible job of protecting the mitochondria but somehow prevent the ROS generated in exercise from triggering the exercise benefit - worst of all possible outcomes.  This may explain why a recent study of antioxidants in humans found them to be harmful (esp. Vit E, but also others) to overall lifespan.

There are several antioxidants which protect mitochondria, all of which are avaliable as supplements:

1. CoQ10

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.07.012

2. Chlorophyllin

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0300-483X(00)00278-X

3. Carnitine and Lipoic Acid

http://pmid.us/16802113

4. Quercetin 

http://www.wellnessresources.com/studies/entry/quercetin_protects_mitochondria

None of which extend life notably in healthy mice. e.g. see this null result for CoQ10:

http://pmid.us/16443163

So there’s nothing really great available.  Resveratrol, which is among other things an antioxidant, has some weak evidence of providing a net longevity benefit:

In the only positive human trial, extremely high doses (3–5 g) of resveratrol in a proprietary formulation have been necessary to significantly lower blood sugar.[2] Despite mainstream press alleging resveratrol’s anti-aging effects,[3] there is little present scientific basis for the application of these claims to mammals

Red grapes have the most resveratrol; blueberries have 1/10th the amount (blueberries have some other compound, thought to be brain-protective)

Autophagy removes cell components - including ROS-damaged proteins and organelles - by engulfing and digesting them, producing wastes and recycled nutrients. … Upregulating autophagy [has] extraordinarily wide-ranging benefits. Interventions that extend healthy lifespan in animal models include calorie restriction, resveratrol, spermidine, and rapamycin, and in each operates, at least in part, through autophagy. Upregulating autophagy has positive effects in models of several specific neurodegenerative diseases, too … antioxidants inhibit basal autophagy and block the induction of autophagy by calorie restriction and other means. Because this effect inhibits the central mechanism of cell repair, it helps explain why dietary antioxidants have failed to deliver their expected benefits to health and longevity.” I would have said it has more to do with failing to target mitochondria, given the benefits demonstrated by mitochondrially targeted antioxidants. As Drexler notes, however, there’s research to back up the antioxidant-autophagy link, which may have some relation to earlier research showing antioxidant supplementation to interfere with the processes of hormesis, and thus block beneficial effects of mild stress such as exercise. 

Autophagy is good (if you want to live long).  Antioxidants also generally interfere with that.

Rapamycin is immunosuppresive and so not of practical interest (unless you want to live in a bubble).

Spermadine (exact mechanism unknown) increases autophagy in cultured cells, and lifespan in yeast, nematodes, and flies.

Fun

1161
1171
1158
1147
1138
1134
1116

[The] 15 Best Things About …

This sign could use a “the” and another “l”:

15best

AD36 (Cold Virus) Exposure Makes You Fat, Maybe

Criticisms of the recent Gabbert et al AD36obesity paper:

(found via http://scienceblogs.com/erv/2010/09/we_are_exposed_to_more_viruses_1.php ):

(also, another study of mostly-fit and all-exercising army adults finds no link between AD36 and obesity)

The data indicated that for the AD36 Negative group, the ages were 8-11 yrs (18%), 12-15 yrs (65%), and 15-18 yrs (17%). For the AD36 Positive group, the ages were 8-11 yrs (5%), 12-15 yrs (32%), and 15-18 yrs (63%). This is as we surmised above: more older kids are in the AD36 group. The weight average for the AD36 Negative group was 69 kg (+/- 24 kg SD); for the AD36 Positive group is was 93 kg (+/- 24 kg SD).

Thus, one sure finding is that older kids are heavier: in fact, they were about 24 kg heavier, which translated to about 50 lbs. It is at least good to see that the press release got this figure correct.

Another finding is that older kids are more likely to have been previously infected by AD36, also as we surmised (in two years, you have have plenty of colds).

There is no modeling of the expected distribution of biometric properties of the two populations (AD36-negative and -positive) given the other statistics (age,sex,race) reported. This is extremely surprising to me.

However, it’s not just that older kids (adults, really) weigh more - the size-relative metrics BMI, and waist/height ratio were also higher in the AD36 antibody group. But still, those should be adjusted for age as well; their distribution (and mean) will surely change with age, and definitely will change for the worse with age in the destined-to-be-obese (it takes time to blossom into full adult obesity). The fact that no such adjustment was made means that the study contributes almost no additional evidence, but this could be corrected with a proper analysis. And, supposing the raw data is available, this can happen.

The two populations also have a significant sex difference: the AD36-negative group is 58% male, and the AD36-positive is only 47%. However, it looks like adult men and women have similar recommended BMI and waist/height ratios (actually, women are recommended to have slightly lower) - I don’t know what the actual averages are. Racially, there’s a 13% shift from “non-hispanic white” to “hispanic” in the AD36-positive population. While I don’t know how different those groups are re: AD36-positivity or BMI etc., this should be considered as well. The age mismatch is definitely the most severe problem.

The “discussion” section lists much other work which seems to provide better evidence of a AD36-obesity link (I assume the authors are leaving out any negative results that don’t support their views). For example:

[In] a small substudy of adult twins with discordant AD36-specific antibody status, 13 Twins with antibodies to AD36 were noted to have higher BMI values and greater proportions of body fat then their respective antibody-negative twins.

(evidence of correlation, not causality - would be quite strong evidence if the number of twins and BMI/body fat mismatch were large enough)

infection of nonhuman primates, rodents, and chickens with AD36 increased total body fat independent of energy intake

(causality in animal models, although i would hope for exposure rather than infection as the trigger - perhaps p(infection|exposure) is high enough that it doesn’t matter)

adipose-derived stem/stromal cells … infected with AD36 showed increased differentiation and higher levels of lipid accumulation than noninfected control cells

(causal, but in vitro: doesn’t guarantee net fattening in the context of a human body)

 

Safe Sex

from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Aids#Prevention

Estimated per act risk for acquisition

of HIV by exposure route (US only) [80] Exposure Route Estimated infections
per 10,000 exposures
to an infected source Blood Transfusion 9,000[81] Childbirth (to child) 2,500[61] Needle-sharing injection drug use 67[82] Percutaneous needle stick 30[83] Receptive anal intercourse* 50[84][85] Insertive anal intercourse* 6.5[84][85] Receptive penile-vaginal intercourse* 10[84][85][86] Insertive penile-vaginal intercourse* 5[84][85] Receptive oral intercourse 1[85] Insertive oral intercourse 0.5[85] * assuming no condom use
§ source refers to oral intercourse
performed on a man

So the reason to use a condom is to prevent insemination, and the spread of the much more prevalent and virulent STDs like chlamydia and gonorrhea.  I guess HIV is scarier because it’s less curable, and often leads to AIDS, which is usually fatal.

Fertile Women

In Study 1, the scent of women near peak levels of fertility heightened the men’s implicit accessibility to sexual concepts. Study 2 demonstrated that, among men who reported being particularly sensitive to odors, scent cues of fertility triggered heightened perceptions of women’s sexual arousal. Study 3 revealed that in a face-to-face interaction, cues of fertility increased men’s tendency to make risky decisions and to behaviorally mimic a female partner. …

Whereas women may have been selected to suppress cues of ovulation in order to sustain men’s commitment, men have been selected to identify fertility cues in order to enhance a short-term mating endeavor’s probability of reproductive success. … It is unlikely that all indicators of fertility could be suppressed, because some detectable shifts in hormones are needed to facilitate ovulation. Consequently, men must rely on fairly subtle cues (e.g., changes in scent and skin tone) associated with those hormonal shifts to help them respond adaptively to women’s changing levels of fertility.

A 

book review 
in the latest Quarterly Review of Biology:

The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality. [2008] … Thornhill and Gangestad argue that [human] women possess two distinct evolved sexualities. One is the “extended sexuality” that women engage in when conception is impossible; the other they call “estrous” sexuality. The former functions to elicit “material benefits” from males, the latter to acquire “good genes” for offspring, and in keeping with these distinct functions, candidate male partners are evaluated on distinct criteria in the two contexts.

Cheating happens.  Men are more likely to push for it with a fertile woman, and woman are more likely to desire it when fertile.

Reducing Stress and Increasing Testosterone by Holding Physically Expansive Poses in Front of Others

In their article, to be published in a forthcoming

Psychological Science, Cuddy and coauthors Dana R. Carney and Andy J. Yap of Columbia University detail the results of an experiment in which forty-two male and female participants were randomly assigned to a high- or low-power pose group. No one was told what the study was about; instead, each participant believed it was related to the placement of ECG electrodes above and below his or her heart.

Subjects in the high-power group were manipulated into two expansive poses for one minute each: first, the classic feet on desk, hands behind head; then, standing and leaning on one’s hands over a desk. Those in the low-power group were posed for the time period in two restrictive poses: sitting in a chair with arms held close and hands folded, and standing with arms and legs crossed tightly. Saliva samples taken before and after the posing measured testosterone and cortisol levels. To evaluate risk tolerance, participants were given $2 and told they could roll a die for even odds of winning $4. Finally, participants were asked to indicate how “powerful” and “in charge” they felt on a scale from one to four.

Controlling for subjects’ baseline levels of both hormones, Cuddy and her coauthors found that high-power poses decreased cortisol by about 25 percent and increased testosterone by about 19 percent for both men and women. In contrast, low-power poses increased cortisol about 17 percent and decreased testosterone about 10 percent.

Not surprisingly, high-power posers of both sexes also reported greater feelings of being powerful and in charge. In addition, those in the high-power group were more likely to take the risk of gambling their $2; 86 percent rolled the die in the high-power group as opposed to 60 percent of the low-power posers.

Previous research established that situational role changes can cause shifts in hormone levels. In primate groups, for example, after an alpha male dies the testosterone levels of the animal replacing him go up. The hormonal shifts measured in this experiment show that such changes can be influenced independent of role, situation, or any consciously focused thoughts about power. The physical poses are enough.

Does being told to take such poses explain the effect? Does taking the poses voluntarily in solitude preserve the effect? Or is it only taking the pose in an environment where you’re watched by others?

(Some) Alcohol Is Good for You (at Least if You’re Old)

Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research -

Background: Growing epidemiological evidence indicates that moderate alcohol consumption
is associated with reduced total mortality among middle-aged and older adults. However, the salutary effect of moderate drinking may be overestimated owing to confounding factors. Abstainers
may include former problem drinkers with existing health problems and may be atypical compared to drinkers in terms of sociodemographic and social-behavioral factors. The purpose of this
study was to examine the association between alcohol consumption and all-cause mortality over
20 years among 1,824 older adults, controlling for a wide range of potential confounding factors
associated with abstention.

Methods: The sample at baseline included 1,824 individuals between the ages of 55 and 65.
The database at baseline included information on daily alcohol consumption, sociodemographic
factors, former problem drinking status, health factors, and social-behavioral factors. Abstention
was defined as abstaining from alcohol at baseline. Death across a 20-year follow-up period was
confirmed primarily by death certificate.

Results: Controlling only for age and gender, compared to moderate drinkers, abstainers had
a more than 2 times increased mortality risk, heavy drinkers had 70% increased risk, and light
drinkers had 23% increased risk. A model controlling for former problem drinking status, existing
health problems, and key sociodemographic and social-behavioral factors, as well as for age and
gender, substantially reduced the mortality effect for abstainers compared to moderate drinkers.
However, even after adjusting for all covariates, abstainers and heavy drinkers continued to show
increased mortality risks of 51 and 45%, respectively, compared to moderate drinkers
.


Also, Animal Kingdom is the best movie I’ve seen in the last few years.

Erroneous Proof of NP=PSPACE

I was excited to see a claim that NP=PSPACE (pdf) on the Theory of Computing Blog Aggregator.

Unfortunately, Lemma 3 is in error.  A simple counterexample: the QBF “Ay Ex p(x,y)” where p(x,y)=”(x or not(y)) and (not(x) or y)” is predicted by Lemma 3 to be equivalent to “Ex x and not(x)” which is false.  (A means for all, E means exists).  But suppose y is false - then x=false makes p(x,y) true.  Alternatively, let y be true - then x=true makes p(x,y) true.  This means that for all y, there’s an x such that p(x,y), which is the original QBF.

For PSPACE to be in NP, you would need to be able to verify short proofs (of polynomial size and in polynomial time) verifying the correctness of a program’s output (provided the program uses space bounded by some polynomial of the input size).  That would be amazing.

Sales Pitch for AI (1955)

We propose that a 2 month, 10 man study of artificial intelligence be carried out during the summer of 1956 at Dartmouth College in Hanover, New Hampshire. The study is to proceed on the basis of the conjecture that every aspect of learning or any other feature of intelligence can in principle be so precisely described that a machine can be made to simulate it. An attempt will be made to find how to make machines use language, form abstractions and concepts, solve kinds of problems now reserved for humans, and improve themselves. We think that a significant advance can be made in one or more of these problems if a carefully selected group of scientists work on it together for a summer.

(McCarthy et al. 1955)


It takes me two months to implement and test a simple machine learning algorithm.  Those were some smart dudes.

People Narrowly Escaping Disaster

Hand-washing


This is hilarious.  Hand-washing apparently has profound psychological effects, which are usually purported to be metaphorical (“Out, damn’d spot!”).  But this leaves me dying for experimental clarification -what happens to the postdecisional dissonance if the subjects instead:

1) “wash” their hands in some staining, sticky, or malodorous substance?

2) are told (prior or post ablution) that  the soap was contaminated with some pathogen, so they need to wash again to be safe?

3) rub their hands together vigorously without any fluid or soap?

4) play a video game?

(and for longer and shorter durations, of course)



Science 7 May 2010:
Vol. 328. no. 5979, p. 709
DOI: 10.1126/science.1186799


BREVIA

Washing Away Postdecisional Dissonance

Spike W. S. Lee* and Norbert Schwarz
After choosing between two alternatives, people perceive the chosen alternative as more attractive and the rejected alternative as less attractive. This postdecisional dissonance effect was eliminated by cleaning one’s hands. Going beyond prior purification effects in the moral domain, physical cleansing seems to more generally remove past concerns, resulting in ametaphorical “clean slate” effect.
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA.

* To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: spikelee@umich.edu

Hand washing removes more than dirt—it also removes the guilt of past misdeeds, weakens the urge to engage in compensatory behavior (1), and attenuates the impact of disgust on moral judgment (2). These findings are usually conceptualized in terms of a purity-morality metaphor that links physical and moral cleanliness (3); however, they may also reflect that washing more generally removes traces of the past by metaphorically wiping the slate clean. If so, washing one’s hands may lessen the influence of past behaviors that have no moral implications at all. We test this possibility in a choice situation. Freely choosing between two similarly attractive options (e.g., Paris or Rome for vacation) arouses cognitive dissonance, an aversive psychological state resulting from conflicting cognitions. Peoplereduce dissonance by perceiving the chosen alternative as more attractive and the rejected alternative as less attractive after choice, thereby justifying their decision (4, 5). We test whether hand washing reduces this classic postdecisional dissonance effect.

In individual sessions as part of an alleged consumer survey, 40 undergraduates browsed 30 CD covers as if they were in a music store. They selected 10 CDs they would like to own and ranked them by preference. Later, the experimenter offered them a choice between their fifth- and sixth-ranked CDs as a token of appreciation from the sponsor. After the choice, participants completed an ostensibly unrelated product survey that asked for evaluations of a liquid soap; half merely examined the bottle before answering, whereas others tested the soap by washing their hands. After a filler task, participants ranked the 10 CDs again, allegedly because the sponsor wanted to know whatpeople think about the CDs after leaving the store (6).

Can washing one’s hands attenuate the need to justify a recent choice? Yes (Fig. 1). For those who merely examined the soap, the preference for the chosen over the rejected alternative increased from before choice [mean (M) = 0.14 and SD = 1.01] to after choice (M = 2.05, SD = 1.96) by an average of 1.9 ranks [F(1, 38) = 20.40, P  the standard dissonance effect. In contrast, for those who washed their hands, preferences were unaffected by their decision [before choice, M = 0.68, SD = 0.75; after choice, M = 1.00, SD = 1.41; F  reduced the need to justify one’s choice by increasing the perceived difference between alternatives [F(1, 38) = 6.74, P = 0.01, for the interaction of time and hand-washing manipulation].

328_709_f1

  

 
Fig. 1. Postdecisional dissonance after hand washing or no hand washing (study 1). Each bar represents the rank difference between the chosen and rejected alternatives, with higher values indicating higher preferences for the chosen alternative. Error bars represent standard error.
 
A study with a different choice task, cleaning manipulation, and measure replicated this finding (7). In individual sessions, 85 students responded to an alleged consumer survey about four fruit jams (shown in pictures). They were subsequently offered a choice between two jars as a sign of the sponsor’s appreciation. After their choice, participants completed an ostensibly unrelated product survey about an antiseptic wipe; half merely examined the wipe, whereas others tested it by cleaning their hands. Next, they rated the expected taste of the four jams (0 = notgood at all; 10 = very good).

Participants who did not clean their hands after making a choice expected the chosen jam to taste much better (M = 8.00, SD = 1.65) than the rejected jam (M = 6.43, SD = 1.81) [F(1, 83) = 27.54, P  attenuated this difference to nonsignificance [for chosen jam, M = 7.63 and SD = 1.56; for rejected jam,M = 7.23 and SD = 1.25; F(1, 83) = 1.79, P = 0.19, for the simple effect]. Thus, hand cleaning significantly reduced the classic postdecisional dissonance effect [F(1, 83) = 7.80, P = 0.006, for the interaction of product and hand-cleaning manipulation].

These findings indicate that the psychological impact of physical cleansing extends beyond the moral domain. Much as washing can cleanse us from traces of past immoral behavior, it can also cleanse us from traces of past decisions, reducing the need to justify them. This observation is not captured by the purity-morality metaphor and highlights the need for a better understanding of the processes that mediate the psychological impact of physicalcleansing. To further constrain the range of plausible candidate explanations, future research may test whether the observed “clean slate” effect is limited to past acts that may threaten one’s self-view (e.g., moral transgressions and potentially poor choices) or also extends to past behaviors with positive implications.

Supporting Online Material

www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5979/709/DC1

Materials and Methods

References and Notes 


References and Notes
  • 1. C.-B. Zhong, K. Liljenquist, Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science 313, 1451 (2006). [Abstract/Free Full Text]
  • 2. S. Schnall, J. Benton, S. Harvey, With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments.Psychol. Sci. 19, 1219 (2008). [Free Full Text]
  • 3. G. Lakoff, M. Johnson, Philosophy in the Flesh (Basic, New York, 1999).
  • 4. L. Festinger, A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Stanford Univ. Press, Stanford, CA, 1957).
  • 5. J. Cooper, Cognitive Dissonance: 50 Years of a Classic Theory (Sage, London, 2007).
  • 6. S. Kitayama, A. C. Snibbe, H. R. Markus, T. Suzuki, Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychol. Sci. 15, 527 (2004). [Free Full Text]
  • 7. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online.
  • 8. We thank the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences and the R. C. Lee Charitable Foundation for support.
  •  

     

    Kick Ass, Solar, House, Black Dynamite, Extremely Loud, House, Ironman 2, Alice in Wonderland

    Kick Ass: excellent. alternatively hilarious and moving. almost ruined by retarded climactic “boss fight”.

    Solar: excellent. both slapstick and insightful.  includes brutal mockery of (strawman?) postmodernism.  same author as book behind movie “Atonement”, which I also liked.

    House: pretty much the only soap opera I’ve ever enjoyed, in spite of some “repetitive formula” episodes.

    Black Dynamite: funny

    Extremely Loud and Incredibly Close: good. overly sentimental and leans too much on the Forrest Gump idiot/outsider child mouthpiece for the author’s cleverest observations, but also surprisingly touching by the end.

    Ironman 2: boring and dumb.  high quality crap.

    Alice and Wonderland: extremely boring and occasionally charming

    Biases Pamphlet

    http://www.scribd.com/doc/30548590/Cognitive-Biases-A-Visual-Study-Guide-by-t…
    ( pdf: http://www.isi.edu/~graehl/Cognitive-Biases-A-Visual-Study-Guide-by-the-Royal… )

    I wonder if it’s worth memorizing the name+summary of “the 42 decision-making biases”, “the 19 social biases”, etc. Broadly, biases benefit us by providing:

    1) Believable signaling. We’re often our only advocate. In order to make people trust and like us, we have to promote ourselves. But deception is most effective when we don’t consciously believe we’re lying (few people are excellent face-to-face fakers). Biases that help us sell ourselves, or convince the in-group that we’re loyal to it, will help us.

    2) Reasonable use of limited computational resources. We employ heuristics that focus our attention, or accelerate a decision, and we should expect those to serve reasonably well in the social+physical environment we evolved in, but to be less accurate than full consideration. That is, our cognitive architecture is mostly satisfactory (but not necessarily optimal) for making decisions given limited computational power and time. It amuses me that it’s so unusual to really think through a problem in a truly rational and systematic way.

    One error I noticed: “moral credential effect” is broader than prejudice. It applies quite generally; if you think you’ve taken a morally good action today, you’re more likely to cheat elsewhere.

    There are many overlaps (and several repetitions of some items under different categories): e.g. “choice-supportive bias” overlaps “self-serving bias” “dunning-kruger” and “hindsight bias”.

    Corn Syrup Hysteria

    Princeton scientists have been claiming that any food that has more fructose than glucose is harmful and obesity-promoting.  Table sugar and bananas, for example, have equal parts fructose and glucose.  HFCS has slightly more fructose than glucose, and many fruits have significantly more (for instance, apples).

    In studies directly comparing the effect of fructose and glucose preloads on subsequent food intake, one showed no difference [7], while the majority have shown the fructose preload resulting in lesser food intake than the glucose preload [8-10]. A recent review of the literature on fructose’s effect on satiety found no compelling case for the idea that fructose is less satiating than glucose, or that HFCS is less satiating than sucrose [11].
    (from Alan Aragon)

    An apple has 10g of fructose.  There’s no reason to believe that 50g of fructose per day is harmful.

    One of My Favorite Things


    (720p HD please)

    Spinning Universe

    Time lapse of Mauna Kea skies

    Brooklyn’s Finest

    Good acting. Stylish. I liked most of the shots. I loved a moment of contemplative silence (no soundtrack, just the murmur of the streets at night). Several nearly independent storylines, but often boring writing (not as terrible as Avatar). Critical reviews are mostly negative, which seems wrong to me. Training Day (same director) was better, but this was fine.

    Costly Trivia

    The current research tested the hypothesis that making many choices impairs subsequent self-control. Drawing from a limited-resource model of self-regulation and executive function, the authors hypothesized that decision making depletes the same resource used for self-control and active responding. In 4 laboratory studies, some participants made choices among consumer goods or college course options, whereas others thought about the same options without making choices. 
    Making choices led to reduced self-control (i.e., less physical stamina, reduced persistence in the face of failure, more procrastination, and less quality and quantity of arithmetic calculations). A field study then found that reduced self-control was predicted by shoppers’ self-reported degree of previous active decision making. Further studies suggested that choosing is more depleting than merely deliberating and forming preferences about options and more depleting than implementing choices made by someone else and that anticipating the choice task as enjoyable can reduce the depleting effect for the first choices but not for many choices.
     (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved)
    Source: “Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative.” from Journal of Personality and Social Psychology by Vohs, Kathleen D.; Baumeister, Roy F.; Schmeichel, Brandon J.; Twenge, Jean M.; Nelson, Noelle M.; Tice, Dianne M.
    If someone wants to hash out a joint plan with me (“What do you do you want to do?”  “I don’t know, what do you want to do?”), I’ll avoid impatiently truncating the process with an arbitrary choice.  I’m not going to make any decision for the both of us unless I’m sure that they’ll gladly follow my lead.  My suggestions will come with very little internal commitment (“well, I like this place …”).  I want to make each decision at most once.

    Trivia

    Wearing dark glasses makes people think they can get away with cheating. - Darkness Increases Dishonest Behavior

    Lonely people are unhappy, but small talk doesn’t help at all.  Exchange of interesting information (including gossip) makes people happy.  You can expect someone whose socialization is entirely small talk to be as miserable as someone who is completely alone.  Disclaimer: I think you can trust self-descriptions of happiness only as much as the interviewer isn’t someone the subject wants to impress, but perhaps social scientists know how to compensate for that effect - Eavesdropping on (un)happiness