Alcohol (Like Pot) Improves Remote Associates Test Performance

Same as pot (no cite for that - sorry):

New research indicates that alcohol, which many a moody poet has indulged in, may actually increase creativity. Psychologists Andrew F. JaroszGregory J.H. Colflesh, and Jennifer Wiley recruited 40 males, got half of them a little tipsy on vodka, and then subjected them to the “Remote Associates Test,” which tests insightful thinking.  Their results, as summarized by the BPS Research Digest, were surprisingly good:

The key finding of the new research is that the intoxicated participants solved more items on the Remote Associates Test compared with the control participants (they solved 58 per cent of 15 items on average vs. 42 per cent average success achieved by controls), and they tended to solve the items more quickly (11.54 seconds per item vs. 15.24 seconds). Moreover, the intoxicated participants tended to rate their experience of problem solving as more insightful, like an Aha! moment, and less analytic. They also performed worse on a working memory test, as you might expect. 

via

I wonder if increased RAT performance is even remotely a good thing. I think the idea of altered brain states for creativity is plausible (if you’re stuck in a rut and want to explore). I just wonder how to trigger them without causing lasting damage.

Films

Great: Two Lovers
Good: PolisseYou Are the Apple of My Eye, Ocean Heaven
Nice: The Reader
Bad: Goon

Films

good: The Wave
ok: BrothersBoondock Saints, Ricky 6
bad: The Grey (though I so love Liam Neeson), We Bought a Zoo (though I so love Matt Damon)

Memory Is a Series of Hexagonal Tubes

via - hexagonal tubes are 15% of the brain’s protein. Previously, we knew that long term potentiation of synapses features a hexagonal enzyme, CaMKII, which carries 6 bits of information (presence or absence of phosphorus at each of its 6 corners).

The new result is that computer models have shown that the same CaMKII that’s involved in potentiating synapses, can also fit inside the hexagonal “microtubules” and write (or read; probably both - I’m not clear) the 6 phosphorylation bits in a slice of the microtubule. Probably microtubules, which are long, can hold very many 6-bit bytes.

It’s known that long term memories are lost if recalled and not stored. I could imagine the transfer of molecules from microtubules having this characteristic (if you don’t put them back when you’re done, they’re gone).

CaMKII and microtubules exist in non-brain and non-human tissue as well.

Incentive Pay (and Political Pressure) Depresses Crime Reporting in NY

Be careful what you measure and reward:

… send victims to other precincts, discount them because they weren’t totally cooperative, reclassify a crime, delay recording a crime, or reject a crime because they didn’t think prosecutors would pursue a conviction.

These are all dodges that have evolved in the era of CompStat, the NYPD’s widely copied crime-fighting strategy, which ties career promotions to crime numbers, creating a strong incentive for commanders to downgrade reports.

All of which helps Bloomberg’s administration and brings tourism and investment to NYC.

via (also, heinous whistleblower retaliation)

Films

good: The Secret World of Arrietty, 12, Black Book, Croupier

decent: Felon, The Bank Job, You Don’t Know Jack

BPI “Boneless Lean Beef” Trimming-slurry: Avoid Ground Beef

Ground beef is now equally as disgusting as hot dogs. There’s no labeling requirement for this shit.

Of course, it may be perfectly safe, for all I know. But it’s not quality, fresh meat.

via and video

Caffeine Affects Fat Metabolism Like Light Weight Training

via

Several genes involved in fat metabolism that were methylated before the exercise lost their methyl group. Such demethylation allows genes to more easily make proteins, which suggests that more proteins involved in the breakdown of fat are being made after exercise

And in petri dishes, exposure to caffeine caused the same beneficial demethylation, probably via elevation of muscle calcium (the same mechanism that probably explains exercise-caused demethylation.

We already knew from the results that muscle-preserving (at the expense of fat) adaptations were triggered by weight training. What’s interesting is that caffeine triggers some of the same adaptations directly.

Recuperation Aided by Placebo: How?

I just skimmed a long and detailed explanation of the biochemstry of reacting to beliefs about recovery caused by taking fake treatments. The claim is that when we’re seriously ill or injured, we’re either in a resting mode or a fight/flight mode. If we’re still fighting whatever injured us, our body doesn’t budget for healing; it suppresses the pain. If we accept that our role is to be laid up and cared for until we recover, then we budget for it (and feel psychologically unable to exert ourselves hard). A placebo treatment definitely makes it seem like our tribe expects us to rest and recuperate - that we’ll be protected, that it’s safe to make ourselves sluggish and disabled in a way that can’t be immediately reversed.

I’ve noticed the tension between fighting and recuperating, subjectively, in my mild soccer injuries. Initially, I’m not sure about the severity, but for a few seconds at least, I’m unwilling to move at all, until I can take stock. Later, if I decide to keep competing, it takes a while to overcome the resistance to voluntary movement that the pain signals gave me (I doubt my muscles are so functionally disabled by a kick in the shin). Of course, later, when the match is over, I feel more impaired than when I was fighting, and I doubt it’s only because of swelling setting in.

This hypothesis also explains why I’ve had extremely poor results whenever I lifted weights or otherwise exercised extremely hard when in the beginning or middle of a cold/fever, and perhaps how I’ve escaped full symptoms by allowing extra sleep when I feel tired after the scratchy-inflamed-throat beginnings of a cold.

I wish I knew when it was okay to treat symptoms directly (without that suppression of symptoms suppressing necessary responses to the underlying cause). For example, hospitals that medicate to lower fevers of under 104F kill more of their patients than they save.

Habits

The process within our brains that creates habits is a three-step loop. First, there is a cue, a trigger that tells your brain to go into automatic mode and which habit to use. Then there is the routine, which can be physical or mental or emotional. Finally, there is a reward, which helps your brain figure out if this particular loop is worth remembering for the future. Over time, this loop — cue, routine, reward; cue, routine, reward — becomes more and more automatic. The cue and reward become neurologically intertwined until a sense of craving emerges. What’s unique about cues and rewards, however, is how subtle they can be. Neurological studies like the ones in Graybiel’s lab have revealed that some cues span just milliseconds. And rewards can range from the obvious (like the sugar rush that a morning doughnut habit provides) to the infinitesimal (like the barely noticeable — but measurable — sense of relief the brain experiences after successfully navigating the driveway). Most cues and rewards, in fact, happen so quickly and are so slight that we are hardly aware of them at all. But our neural systems notice and use them to build automatic behaviors.

Habits aren’t destiny — they can be ignored, changed or replaced. But it’s also true that once the loop is established and a habit emerges, your brain stops fully participating in decision-making. So unless you deliberately fight a habit — unless you find new cues and rewards — the old pattern will unfold automatically. […]

Luckily, simply understanding how habits work makes them easier to control. Take, for instance, a series of studies conducted a few years ago at Columbia University and the University of Alberta. Researchers wanted to understand how exercise habits emerge. In one project, 256 members of a health-insurance plan were invited to classes stressing the importance of exercise. Half the participants received an extra lesson on the theories of habit formation (the structure of the habit loop) and were asked to identify cues and rewards that might help them develop exercise routines.

The results were dramatic. Over the next four months, those participants who deliberately identified cues and rewards spent twice as much time exercising as their peers. Other studies have yielded similar results. According to another recent paper, if you want to start running in the morning, it’s essential that you choose a simple cue (like always putting on your sneakers before breakfast or leaving your running clothes next to your bed) and a clear reward (like a midday treat or even the sense of accomplishment that comes from ritually recording your miles in a log book). After a while, your brain will start anticipating that reward — craving the treat or the feeling of accomplishment — and there will be a measurable neurological impulse to lace up your jogging shoes each morning.

Our relationship to e-mail operates on the same principle. When a computer chimes or a smartphone vibrates with a new message, the brain starts anticipating the neurological “pleasure” (even if we don’t recognize it as such) that clicking on the e-mail and reading it provides. That expectation, if unsatisfied, can build until you find yourself moved to distraction by the thought of an e-mail sitting there unread — even if you know, rationally, it’s most likely not important. On the other hand, once you remove the cue by disabling the buzzing of your phone or the chiming of your computer, the craving is never triggered, and you’ll find, over time, that you’re able to work productively for long stretches without checking your in-box. […]

When they got back to P.& G.’s headquarters, the researchers watched their videotapes again. Now they knew what to look for and saw their mistake in scene after scene. Cleaning has its own habit loops that already exist. In one video, when a woman walked into a dirty room (cue), she started sweeping and picking up toys (routine), then she examined the room and smiled when she was done (reward). In another, a woman scowled at her unmade bed (cue), proceeded to straighten the blankets and comforter (routine) and then sighed as she ran her hands over the freshly plumped pillows (reward). P.& G. had been trying to create a whole new habit with Febreze, but what they really needed to do was piggyback on habit loops that were already in place. The marketers needed to position Febreze as something that came at the end of the cleaning ritual, the reward, rather than as a whole new cleaning routine.

The company printed new ads showing open windows and gusts of fresh air. More perfume was added to the Febreze formula, so that instead of merely neutralizing odors, the spray had its own distinct scent. Television commercials were filmed of women, having finished their cleaning routine, using Febreze to spritz freshly made beds and just-laundered clothing. Each ad was designed to appeal to the habit loop: when you see a freshly cleaned room (cue), pull out Febreze (routine) and enjoy a smell that says you’ve done a great job (reward). When you finish making a bed (cue), spritz Febreze (routine) and breathe a sweet, contented sigh (reward). Febreze, the ads implied, was a pleasant treat, not a reminder that your home stinks. 

And so Febreze, a product originally conceived as a revolutionary way to destroy odors, became an air freshener used once things are already clean. The Febreze revamp occurred in the summer of 1998. Within two months, sales doubled. A year later, the product brought in $230 million. Since then Febreze has spawned dozens of spinoffs — air fresheners, candles and laundry detergents — that now account for sales of more than $1 billion a year. Eventually, P.& G. began mentioning to customers that, in addition to smelling sweet, Febreze can actually kill bad odors. Today it’s one of the top-selling products in the world. […]

But when some customers were going through a major life event, like graduating from college or getting a new job or moving to a new town, their shopping habits became flexible in ways that were both predictable and potential gold mines for retailers. The study found that when someone marries, he or she is more likely to start buying a new type of coffee. When a couple move into a new house, they’re more apt to purchase a different kind of cereal. When they divorce, there’s an increased chance they’ll start buying different brands of beer.

Consumers going through major life events often don’t notice, or care, that their shopping habits have shifted, but retailers notice, and they care quite a bit. At those unique moments, Andreasen wrote, customers are “vulnerable to intervention by marketers.” In other words, a precisely timed advertisement, sent to a recent divorcee or new homebuyer, can change someone’s shopping patterns for years. […]

Before I met Andrew Pole, before I even decided to write a book about the science of habit formation, I had another goal: I wanted to lose weight.

I had got into a bad habit of going to the cafeteria every afternoon and eating a chocolate-chip cookie, which contributed to my gaining a few pounds. Eight, to be precise. I put a Post-it note on my computer reading “NO MORE COOKIES.” But every afternoon, I managed to ignore that note, wander to the cafeteria, buy a cookie and eat it while chatting with colleagues. Tomorrow, I always promised myself, I’ll muster the willpower to resist.

Tomorrow, I ate another cookie.

When I started interviewing experts in habit formation, I concluded each interview by asking what I should do. The first step, they said, was to figure out my habit loop. The routine was simple: every afternoon, I walked to the cafeteria, bought a cookie and ate it while chatting with friends.

Next came some less obvious questions: What was the cue? Hunger? Boredom? Low blood sugar? And what was the reward? The taste of the cookie itself? The temporary distraction from my work? The chance to socialize with colleagues?

Rewards are powerful because they satisfy cravings, but we’re often not conscious of the urges driving our habits in the first place. So one day, when I felt a cookie impulse, I went outside and took a walk instead. The next day, I went to the cafeteria and bought a coffee. The next, I bought an apple and ate it while chatting with friends. You get the idea. I wanted to test different theories regarding what reward I was really craving. Was it hunger? (In which case the apple should have worked.) Was it the desire for a quick burst of energy? (If so, the coffee should suffice.) Or, as turned out to be the answer, was it that after several hours spent focused on work, I wanted to socialize, to make sure I was up to speed on office gossip, and the cookie was just a convenient excuse? When I walked to a colleague’s desk and chatted for a few minutes, it turned out, my cookie urge was gone.

All that was left was identifying the cue.

Deciphering cues is hard, however. Our lives often contain too much information to figure out what is triggering a particular behavior. Do you eat breakfast at a certain time because you’re hungry? Or because the morning news is on? Or because your kids have started eating? Experiments have shown that most cues fit into one of five categories: location, time, emotional state, other people or the immediately preceding action. So to figure out the cue for my cookie habit, I wrote down five things the moment the urge hit:

Where are you? (Sitting at my desk.)

What time is it? (3:36 p.m.)

What’s your emotional state? (Bored.)

Who else is around? (No one.)

What action preceded the urge? (Answered an e-mail.)

The next day I did the same thing. And the next. Pretty soon, the cue was clear: I always felt an urge to snack around 3:30.

Once I figured out all the parts of the loop, it seemed fairly easy to change my habit. But the psychologists and neuroscientists warned me that, for my new behavior to stick, I needed to abide by the same principle that guided Procter & Gamble in selling Febreze: To shift the routine — to socialize, rather than eat a cookie — I needed to piggyback on an existing habit. So now, every day around 3:30, I stand up, look around the newsroom for someone to talk to, spend 10 minutes gossiping, then go back to my desk. The cue and reward have stayed the same. Only the routine has shifted. It doesn’t feel like a decision, any more than the M.I.T. rats made a decision to run through the maze. It’s now a habit. I’ve lost 21 pounds since then (12 of them from changing my cookie ritual).
 summary via - from an article with linkbait headline I’d avoided reading.

Wine: Ignorance Is Bliss

individuals who are unaware of the price do not derive more enjoyment from more expensive wine.

Study. 

[People] on average enjoy more expensive 
wines slightly  less. For individuals with wine training, however, we find 
indications of a positive relationship between price and enjoyment

Experts have different tastes. So expert recommendations are not useful unless you’re also an expert.

How do you get to be an expert? I presume, by aligning your preferences with those of the experts. 

Ubiquitous Networked Blood Protein Monitors

David Agus has a dream: that devices monitoring the proteins in your bloodstream will be used to prescribe precisely the right medication, and that he’ll be able to read everyone’s data to research optimal treatment advice. Nice idea. I’d participate.

Further, he recommends against multivitamins and antioxidants (which isn’t that controversial; vitamin e and vitamin a are dangerous supplements, as are many things commonly found in multi-vitamins (e.g. manganese) - we get enough in the right form already from diet, and too much of the wrong form is certainly harmful.

He says statins are good for old people - but not because they reduce cholesterol (they do many other things).

He says low-dose aspirin is excellent, because it reduces inflammation (though too much blood-thinning will kill you):

Last year British scientists, looking at eight long-term studies involving 25,000 participants, found that 75 mg a day reduces the risk of dying from common cancers by 10 to 60 per cent.
 
Also, he recommends not sitting for hours straight (walking more often in the workday), not wearing high heels, and a consistent sleep and eating schedule (even over weekends). And fresh-frozen vegetables over all all but the freshest supermarket produce.

Films

good: The Savages (female writer/director; good acting), The Cuckoo
decent: Devils on the DoorstepMalèna, Rabbit Hole

Snorting Insulin Post-meal Reduces Snack Cravings

Study:

Insulin administration in the postprandial but not in the fasted state decreased appetite as well as intake and rated palatability of chocolate chip cookies (the most palatable snack offered). In both experiments, intranasal insulin induced a slight decrease in plasma glucose but did not affect serum insulin concentrations. 

This is snorted (nasal spray) insulin, so it apparently goes to the brain without much affecting overall blood levels.

Normally, too much insulin leads to reduced insulin sensitivity, which is counterproductive, so it’s not clear whether this is a good long term approach. Perhaps the fact that blood insulin levels aren’t elevated doesn’t damage sensitivity.

via (ignore his speculation about high-glycemic starches being a good-for-controlling-snack-cravings diet; that would act through the bloodstream and so isn’t relevant to this study - I’m not saying it’s wrong, just unrelated)

Crappy Old Age Eyesight Causes Crappy Old Age Sleep

At least, it’s an interesting theory.

I bet they can eventually just figure out the right chemicals to take instead of whatever is triggered by your eyes. But maybe some signals go electrically (like all vision) into the brain, in a way that no pill could reproduce.

Vitamin D (2000-6000 IU) taken right when you wake up is supposed to do a lot to normalize the body’s day/night cycle.

via :

The gradual yellowing of the lens and the narrowing of the pupil that occur with age disturb the body’s circadian rhythm, contributing to a range of health problems, these studies suggest. As the eyes age, less and less sunlight gets through the lens to reach key cells in the retina that regulate the body’s circadian rhythm, its internal clock.

“We believe the effect is huge and that it’s just beginning to be recognized as a problem,” said Dr. Patricia Turner, an ophthalmologist in Leawood, Kan., who with her husband, Dr. Martin Mainster, a professor of ophthalmology at the University of Kansas Medical School, has written extensively about the effects of the aging eye on health.

Circadian rhythms are the cyclical hormonal and physiological processes that rally the body in the morning to tackle the day’s demands and slow it down at night, allowing the body to rest and repair. This internal clock relies on light to function properly, and studies have found that people whose circadian rhythms are out of sync, like shift workers, are at greater risk for a number of ailments, including insomnia, heart disease and cancer.

“Evolution has built this beautiful timekeeping mechanism, but the clock is not absolutely perfect and needs to be nudged every day,” said Dr. David Berson, whose lab at Brown University studies how the eye communicates with the brain.

So-called photoreceptive cells in the retina absorb sunlight and transmit messages to a part of the brain called the suprachiasmatic nucleus (S.C.N.), which governs the internal clock. The S.C.N. adjusts the body to the environment by initiating the release of the hormone melatonin in the evening and cortisol in the morning.

Melatonin is thought to have many health-promoting functions, and studies have shown that people with low melatonin secretion, a marker for a dysfunctional S.C.N., have a higher incidence of many illnesses, including cancer, diabetes and heart disease.

It was not until 2002 that the eye’s role in synchronizing the circadian rhythm became clear. It was always believed that the well-known rods and cones, which provide conscious vision, were the eye’s only photoreceptors. But Dr. Berson’s team discovered that cells in the inner retina, called retinal ganglion cells, also had photoreceptors and that these cells communicated more directly with the brain.

These vital cells, it turns out, are especially responsive to the blue part of the light spectrum.

JHU Researcher: Food Is Bad for You (Intermittent Fasting, Calorie Restriction in Humans)

Some people looking to achieve low body fat report doing better intermittently fasting (essentially, skipping breakfast and not eating late at night, which doesn’t seem too hard). Exercise (esp. weight training) would happen after taking BCAA, whey protein, or eating.

Extreme calorie restriction definitely increases lifespan in mice, and many people are speculatively trying it. Of course, the way rats/mice metabolize is quite different from humans.

Fasting every alternate day does seem a hardship (in terms of energy / brain glucose / willpower).

But Mark Mattson @JHU alludes to preliminary human alternate-day-fasting trials - abstract and has definitely proven heart benefits in rats.

Starving yourself on alternate days can make you live longer, according to scientists.
–A group of Americans have said that fasting on and off can boost brain power and help to lose weight at the same time.
–The National Institutes for Aging said their research was based on giving animals the bare minimum of calories required to keep them alive and results showed they lived up to twice as long.
–The diet has since been tested on humans and appears to protect the heart, circulatory system and brain against age-related diseases like Alzheimer’s.

‘Dietery energy restriction extends lifespan and protects the brain and cardiovascular system against age-related disease,’ said Mark Mattson, head of the laboratory of neurosciences at the NIA and professor of neuroscience at John Hopkins University in Baltimore.

‘We have found that dietary energy restriction, particularly when administered in intermittent bouts of major caloric restriction, such as alternative day fasting, activates cellular stress response pathways in neurones,’ he said to the Sunday Times.

In one set of experiments, a group of mice were only fed on alternate days while others were allowed to eat daily.

Both groups were given unlimited access to food on the days they were allowed to eat and eventually consumed the same amount of calories.

Professor Mattson said he found the mice fed on alternate days were more sensitive to insulin and needed to produce less of it.

High levels of the hormone, which is produced to control sugar levels after a meal or snack, are usually associated with lower brain power and are at a higher risk of diabetes.

via 

Memory Erasure Pill

Long term recall (of events, at least) works by destroying the original memory (according to what I read in Brain Rules). In the normal course of things, the memory is refreshed (stored again). Supposedly you can soon take a pill which blocks the saving of memories retrieved. Careful not to think of any fond memories until it wears off :)

via (disclaimer: Wired magazine, so expect techno-hype)

Films

great: The Skin I Live In, The Return
good: Entre les murs (The Class)
fine: A Single Man, Babel, Das Experiment

Charity as Peacocking

Two experiments were undertaken. For the first, 65 men and 65 women, all of an average age of 21, anonymously played a cooperation game where they could donate money via a computer program to a group fund. Donations were selfless acts, as all other players would benefit from the fund, whilst the donor wouldn’t necessarily receive anything in return.

Players did not know who they were playing with. They were observed by either someone of the same sex or opposite sex — two physically attractive volunteers, one man and one woman. Men were found to do significantly more good deeds when observed by the opposite sex. Whilst the number of good deeds made by women didn’t change, regardless of who observed.

For the second experiment, groups of males were formed. Males were asked to make a number of public donations. These increased when observed by an attractive female, where they were found to actively compete with one another. When observed by another male, however, donations didn’t increase.

paper

via 

Fetal Ultrasound Risks

We conclude that ultrasound exposure in fetal life increases the risk of left-handedness in men, suggesting that prenatal ultrasound affects the fetal brain.

Obviously the effects may be good or ill, but I’ll presuppose they’re bad on the face of it (more ways to break an egg than strengthen it).

This may be due to a small portion of improperly administered ultrasound only (during the introductory/clinical phase, ultrasound didn’t increase left-handedness; after wider deployment, it did (1.32x more likely than without ultrasound) - see Seth Roberts’ discussion.

Ultrasound may be essentially completely safe for a fetus at correct doses. Also, I don’t know how often some beneficial medical intervention is indicated by ultrasound. If it were me, I’d ask to see records of the maintenance/calibration of the ultrasound, and limit it to windows where there’s a possible change in treatment (benefit). Curiosity doesn’t seem reason enough.